Background. Machiavellianism is viewed as a manipulative personal mindset, consistently connected to failure of self-awareness and destruction of close interpersonal relationships, which finds indirect proof in the studies of Machiavellianism and other traits of the Dark Triad in healthy population. Additionally, the role of Machiavellianism in the regulation of various forms of social behaviour characterized by various degrees of regulation, complexity, emotional closeness is not yet studied well enough. Machiavellianism and other forms of manipulative behaviour are expected to be viewed as adaptive strategies in regulating interpersonal relationships in situations of uncertainty, will be expressed differently in the relatively normal and clinical populations depending on emotional engagement, tolerance to uncertainty and values.
Objective. The objective of the study was to identify the input in the expression of Machiavellianism, estimated by Mach-4 scale (by R. Christie, F. Geis, adapted by V.V. Znakov, 2000), of such psychological features as empathy (Measure of Emotional Empathy, by A. Mehrabian, N. Epstein, adapted by U. Orlov, U. Emelianov, 1986), attitude to uncertainty (A New Questionnaire of Uncertainty Tolerance/ Intolerance, by T.V. Kornilova), value orientations (Justice-Care Questionnaire, by S. V. Molchanov, 2005), as well as clinical schizotypal traits (SPQ-74, by Raine A., adapted by S.N. Enikolopov and A.G. Efremov, 2001).
Design. The sample included 80 patients with schizophrenia disorders (F 20.0, F 25.0, F 21.0) and 40 participants without psychiatric diagnoses. The patients were divided into two groups. In the group of patients with paranoid schizophrenia there were males (50%) and females (50%); in the group of patients with schizotypal disorders there were males (45%) and females (55%). The participants without psychiatric diagnoses included males (37.5%) and females (62.5%). The mean age of the participants was 30±5. Each patient was undergoing individual research. At the moment of the study, the patients were hospitalized in the Mental Health Research Center, Moscow, Russia. Results. The regression analysis showed that the lack of empathy, i.e. emotional engagement in the process of interpersonal communication was one of the main regulators of Machiavellianism both in the control (p<.05) and the clinical (p<.001) groups. At the same time, the pronounced schizotypal traits, which are connected to emotional relational aspect of clinical symptoms (suspicion, restricted affect, social anxiety), characterized the climate of the interpersonal communication. Being not the leading ones, they provided the basis for exacerbation of manipulative tendencies. In the control group, “interpersonal intolerance to uncertainty” was the additional factor of Machiavellianism indicator (p<.05). Those patients who have difficulty bearing the ambiguity of relationships were prone to manipulate others. Low “intolerance to uncertainty” in the clinical groups may also strengthen the Machiavellianism indicator (p<.01), which is proved by the links with some aspects of moral self-awareness, based on disregard of traditional ethical norms, laws and orders in decision-making in interpersonal communication.
Conclusion. Among the studied parameters, the regulation of Machiavellianism expression both in clinical and in the control groups is determined by empathy and intolerance to uncertainty. Value priorities and clinical traits, while connected to Machiavellianism, do not allow the authors to conclude on the amount of input of the specific indicator on the manipulative behaviour. The discovered constellations of dyadic relationship quality of Machiavellianism may help to elaborate individual strategies of effective communication with patients with schizophrenia disorders in the framework of psychotherapeutic and rehabilitative programs.
PDF: Download
Keywords: machiavellianism; empathy; tolerance to ambiguity; moral consciousness; schizophrenia disorders;
Available Online 31.12.2019
Table 1. Differences between groups based on average Machiavellian scores
Group |
Mean Mach-4 scale |
Group |
Mean Mach-4 scale |
t-criterion |
Patients with paranoid schizophrenia |
71,15 |
Patients with schizotypal disorders |
81,05 |
-2,838** |
Patients with schizotypal disorders |
81,05 |
Control Group |
74,80 |
1,860 |
Control Group |
74,80 |
Patients with paranoid schizophrenia |
71,15 |
-1,334 |
NB. Italics highlight the value at the level of the trend (p = 0.067); ** - significance level p <0.01.
Table 2. Results of correlation analysis in patients with paranoid schizophrenia
Scale |
Restricted Value |
Empathy |
Machiavellianism |
Based on Law and Order |
Strange speech |
0,471** |
-0,358* |
-0,066 |
0,042 |
Social anxiety |
0,470** |
-0,393* |
0,086 |
0,047 |
Interpersonal Relationship Factor |
0,585** |
-0,345* |
-0,136 |
0,155 |
Empathy |
-0,391* |
1 |
-0,334* |
0,119 |
Taking into account the rights and freedoms of another person |
-0,272 |
0,480** |
-0,412** |
0,448** |
Machiavellianism |
0,077 |
-0,334* |
1 |
-0,410** |
Lack of close friends |
0,567** |
-0,385* |
-0,024 |
-0,041 |
NB. The table shows the correlation coefficients. Significant correlations
Table. 3. Results of correlation analysis in patients with schizotypal disorders.
Scales |
Machiavellianism |
TUN |
ITUN |
Instrumental Exchange |
Ideas of Relationship |
Empathy |
Suspicion |
Empathy |
-0.556** |
-0.065 |
0.158 |
-0.438** |
0.200 |
1 |
-0.358* |
ITUN |
-0.338* |
-0.007 |
1 |
0.042 |
-0.170 |
0.158 |
-0.076 |
Suspicion |
0.373* |
-0.452** |
-0.076 |
0.214 |
0.342* |
-0.358* |
1 |
Reliance on law and order |
-0.372* |
0.123 |
0.458* |
0.073 |
-0.167 |
0.286 |
0.196 |
Desire to be nice with others |
-0.219 |
0.086 |
0.527** |
0.059 |
0.005 |
0.145 |
-0.024 |
I-centred and focused on one’s interests |
0.352* |
-0.281 |
-0.126 |
0.108 |
0.252 |
-0.324* |
0.412** |
Taking into account rights and freedoms of another person |
-0.196 |
0.159 |
0.091 |
0.005 |
-0.336* |
0.278 |
-0.292 |
Interpersonal Intolerance to Uncertainty |
0.039 |
-0.084 |
0.295 |
0.523** |
0.009 |
-0.385* |
0.139 |
Pre-Conventional Level of Moral Consciousness |
0.028 |
0.097 |
0.202 |
0.839** |
-0.035 |
0.366* |
0.175 |
Absence of Close Friends |
0.282 |
-0.132 |
0.069 |
0.330* |
0.070 |
-0.361* |
0.403** |
Legend: TUN – Tolerance of Uncertainty. ITUN – Intolerance of Uncertainty.
Table 4. Results of correlation analysis in the control group
Scales |
Machiavellianism |
Suspicion |
0.454** |
Instrumental Exchange |
0.321* |
Taking into account rights and freedoms of another person |
-0.356* |
Post-Conventional Level of Moral Consciousness |
-0.399* |
NB Table 3 uses correlation co-efficient *- р<0.01. *- р
Bauman Z. (2008). Flowing modernity. SPb., Peter.
Beller, J. & Bosse, S. (2017). Machiavellianism has a dimensional latent structure: Results from taxometric analyses. Personality and Individual Differences, 113, 57–62. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.03.014
Brewer, G. & Abell, L. (2017). Machiavellianism, Relationship Satisfaction, and Romantic Relationship Quality. Europe's Journal of Psychology, 13(3), 491–502. doi:10.5964/ejop.v13i3.1217
Carre J.R. & Jones D.N. (2017). Decision Making, Morality, and Machiavellianism: The Role of Dispositional Traits in Gist Extraction. Review of general psychology, 21(1), 23–29. doi: 10.1037/gpr0000093
Castille C.M., Buckner E.V. & Thoroughgood C.N. (2018). Prosocial Citizens Without a Moral Compass? Examining the Relationship Between Machiavellianism and Unethical Pro-Organizational Behavior. Journal of business ethics, 149(4), 919–930. doi: 10.1007/s10551-016-3079-9
Efremov A.G., & Enikolopov S.N. (2001). Testing the methodology for identifying the severity of schizotypal personality traits (SPQ-74). [Materialy Pervoy Mezhdunarodnoy konferentsii, posvyashchennoy pamyati B.V. Zeygarnik]. Moscow, Tsentr SMI MGU, 109–112.
Egorova M.S. (2009). Machiavellianism in the structure of personal properties. [Vestnik Permskogo gosudarstvennogo pedagogicheskogo universiteta]. Series 10, Differential Psychology, 1(2), 65–80.
Gayle, B. & Loren, A. (2017). Machiavellianism, Relationship Satisfaction, and Romantic Relationship Quality. Journal of Psychology,13(3), 491–502. doi: 10.5964/ejop.v13i3.1217
Kobzova M.P. (2015). The dynamics of cognitive functions and social adaptation in patients with schizotypal disorder. Meditsinskaya psikhologiya v Rossii: http://www.medpsy.ru/mprj/archiv_global/2015_1_30/nomer08.php (accessed: 07.10.2019).
Kornilova T.V., & Chigrinova I.A. (2014). Personal values, moral development and emotional intelligence in the regulation of choice in a situation of interpersonal interaction. [Psikhologiya. Zhurnal Vysshey SHkoly Ekonomiki], 11(4), 56–74.
Kornilova T.V. (2010). A new questionnaire of tolerance to uncertainty [Psikhologicheskiy Zhurnal], 31(1), 74–86.
Lang, A. (2018). Mama Mach and Papa Mach: Parental Machiavellianism in Relation to Dyadic Coparenting and Adolescents’ Perception of Parental Behaviour. Europe's Journal of Psychology, 14(1), 107–124. doi:10.5964/ejop.v14i1.1474
Maples-Keller, J. L. & Miller, J. D. (2018). Insight and the Dark Triad: Comparing Self- and Meta-perceptions in Relation to Psychopathy, Narcissism, and Machiavellianism. Personality disorders-theory research and treatment, 9(1), 30–39. doi: 10.1037/per0000207
Molchanov S.V. (2005). The structure of moral behaviour in the concept of J. Rest. [Psikhologiya I shkola], 1, 111–132.
Moskacheva M.A., Kholmogorova A.B., & Garanyan N.G. (2014). Alexithymia and the ability to empathy. [Konsul'tativnaya psikhologiya i psikhoterapiya], 22(4), 98–114.
Osumi, T., Tsuji, K., Shibata, M. & Umeda, S. (2019). Machiavellianism and early neural responses to others; facial expressions caused by one`s own decisions. Psychiatry research, 271, 669–677. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.12.037
Ouellet C., Langlois F., Provencher M.D. & Gosselin P. (2019). Intolerance of uncertainty and difficulties in emotion regulation: Proposal for an integrative model of generalized anxiety disorder. Revue europeenne de psychologie appliqué, 69, 9–18. doi: 10.1016/j.erap.2019.01.001
Pajevic M., Vukosavljevic-Gvozden T., Stevanovic N. & Neumann C.S. (2018). The relationship between the Dark Tetrad and a two-dimensional view of empathy. Personality and Individual Differences, 123, 125–130. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.11.009
Quid Y., Cohen-Woods S., O’Reilly N., Carr V. J., Elzinga B.M. & Green M.J. (2018). Schizotypal personality traits and social cognition are associated with childhood trauma exposure. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57, 397–419. doi:10.1111/bjc.12187
Savulich G., Jeanes, H., Rossides, N., Kaur, S., Zacharia, A., Robbins, T., & Sahakian, B. (2018). Moral emotions and social economic games in paranoia. Frontiers in psychiatry, 9, 615. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00615
Sokolova E.T. (2014). The loss of self: a clinic or a new cultural norm. [Epistemologiya i filosofskie nauki]., 41(3), 190–210. URL : http://psystudy.ru/index.php/num/2015v8n40/1113-sokolova40.html – (дата обращения 07.10.2019).
Sokolova E.T. (2015). The shock of a collision with sociocultural uncertainty: a clinical view. [Psikhologicheskie issledovaniya, 8, 40. URL : http://psystudy.ru/index.php/num/2015v8n40/1113-sokolova40.html (accessed: 07.10.2019).
Sokolova E.T., & Laysheva G.A. (2017) Psychological manipulation as a cultural and clinical phenomenon. [Voprosy psikhologii]. 1, 54–67.
Sokolova E.T., & Andreiuk K.O. (2018). The influence of manipulative attitudes on the mental state of patients with schizotypal disorders. [Kul'turno-istoricheskaya psikhologiya], 14(1), 78–86. doi: 10.17759/chp.2018140109
Sokolova E.T. (2018). Uncertainty and diffusion of identity: cultural, clinical and ethical perspectives. [Istoriya i filosofiya nauki v epokhu peremen: Trudy Pervogo Kongressa ROINF "Istoriya i filosofiya nauki v epokhu peremen]. Moscow, Izdatel’stvo Russkoe obshchestvo istorii i filosofii nauki, Russian Society for the History and Philosophy of Science Moscow, 6, 9–13. Retrieved from: http://rshps.ru/books/congress2018t6.pdf. (accessed: 07.10.2019).
Soldatova G.U., & L.A. Scheigerova (Eds.) (2008). Psychodiagnostics of personality tolerance. Moscow, Smysl, 72–75.
Vonk, J., Zeigler-Hill V., Ewing D., Mercer S. & Noser A. E. (2015). Mindreading in the dark: Dark personality features and theory of mind. Personality and Individual Differences, 87, 50–54. doi:87:505410.1016/j.paid.2015.07.025
Wu W., Wang, H., Zheng, C. & Wu, Y.J. (2019). Effect of Narcissism, Psychopathy, and Machiavellianism on Entrepreneurial Intention-The Mediating of Entrepreneurial Self-Efficacy. Frontiers in psychology, 10, 360. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00360
Wu W., Wang H., Lee H.-Y., Lin Y.-T. & Guo F. (2019) How Machiavellianism, Psychopathy, and Narcissism Affect Sustainable Entrepreneurial Orientation: The Moderating Effect of Psychological Resilience. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 779. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00
Znakov V.V. (2000). Machiavellianism: the psychological property of a person and the methodology of his research. [Psikhologicheskiy Zhurnal], 1(5), 16–22.
Аndreyuk K.O., Sokolova E.T. (2019). Clinical and psychological regulators of Machiavellianism in normal subjects and in patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. National Psychological Journal, [Natsional’nyy psikhologicheskiy zhurnal], (12)4, 35–45.