ISSN 2079-6617
eISSN 2309-9828
Free dance as a cultural-historical practice of improvisation

Free dance as a cultural-historical practice of improvisation

PDF (Rus)

Recieved: 11/04/2020

Accepted: 12/18/2020

Published: 03/30/2021

Keywords: dance practices; improvisation; spontaneity; expressive movement; meaningful attitudes; symbolic movements; pose; live motion

p.: 175-192

DOI: 10.11621/npj.2021.0114

Available online: 30.03.2021

To cite this article:

Ailamazyan Aida M.. Free dance as a cultural-historical practice of improvisation. // National Psychological Journal 2021. 1. p.175-192. doi: 10.11621/npj.2021.0114

Copied to Clipboard

Copy
Issue 1, 2021

Ailamazyan Aida M. Lomonosov Moscow State University

Abstract

Background. Plastic, expressive aspects of human behaviour remain underresearched by psychologists. The focus on practices of improvisation is determined by the fact that they show most vividly how expressive movement comes into being.

Objective. The aim of the study is to provide psychological analysis of improvised dance action, to identify the conditions of its generation. The hypothesis put forward concerns the formation of overall personal attitude that makes one ready to perform expressive movement in the context of musical-motional improvisation. It seems probable that the principles of movement organisation within free dance practices concern the formation of attitude that lets one perceive spontaneous, involuntary impulses to movement, changes of tonus and breath.

Design. The study is a piece of theoretic-psychological analysis of improvisation dance practice. In terms of methodological and theoretical basis the study relies on cultural-historical psychology and theory of action, as well as on N.A. Bernsteins conception of movement building. There theories allow to reconstruct the conditions of expressive movement generation in the context of musical-motional improvisation.

Results. The analysis performed has shown that the principles of movement organisation, the technical aspects of the practices studied are aimed at increasing the degree of freedom of movement. It allows to enhance the receptivity to spontaneous reactions and impulses and to widen the orientation within the context of musical-motional improvisation. It makes one move in a more meaningful way and to integrate the personality into improvisation.

Conclusions. Alongside with the practices of structured dances and reproductive approaches to mastering expressive movement, there are cultural-historical practices of improvisation dances. The analysis of such practices allows to single out psychological conditions and and peculiarities of movement organisation that make one generate spontaneous actions, find and try new objectives, plastic forms. Generation of spontaneous movement and musical-plastic improvisation are possible due to tuning up the whole human personality. Openness as personal attitude has its meaningful as well as motional component.

References

Ainsworth M.D. (1989). Attachment beyond infancy. Amer. Psychology, 44, 709-716. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.44.4.709 

Bennetts S.K. et al. (2017). Mothers’ experiences of parent-reported and video-recorded observational assessments // Journal of Child and Family Studies, 26 (12), 3312-3326. doi:10.1007/sl0826-017-0826-l

Cheng N., Lu S., Archer M., Wang Z. (2018). Quality of Maternal Parenting of 9-Month-Old Infants Predicts Executive Function Performance at 2 and 3 Years of Age. Frontiers in Psychology 8, 2293. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02293

Clark R., Tluczek A., Moore E.C., Evenson A.L. (2019). Theoretical and empirical foundations for early relationship assessment in evaluating infant and toddler mental health. In Rebecca DelCarmen-Wiggins and Alice S. Carter (Eds.). The Oxford handbook of infant, toddler, and preschool mental health assessment,2nd Edition, (pp. 29-50). doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199837182.013.2 

Feniger-Schaal R., Joels T. (2018). Attachment quality of children with ID and its link to maternal sensitivity and structuring. Research in developmental disabilities, 76,56-64. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2018.03.004

Galasjuk I.N., Mitina O.V. (2020). Interaction between parent and young child: structure and dynamics of parental responsiveness. Cultural-historical psychology. [Kulturno-istoricheskaja psihologija],16 (4), 72-86. (in Russ.).

Green J. et al. (2017). Randomised trial of a parent-mediated intervention for infants at high risk for autism: longitudinal outcomes to age 3 years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,58 (12), 1330-1340. doi:10.1111/jcpp.l2728 

Guralnick M. J. (2017). Early Intervention for Children with Intellectual Disabilities : An Update. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities,30,211-229. doi:10.1111/jar.l2233

Hedegaard M., Libert N. (2020). Construction of a Scale for Investigating Pre-School Childrens Social Situations of Development: Focusing on Activities instead of Abilities. Cultural-historical psychology [Kulturno-istoricheskaJa psihologija],16 (2), 25-32. doi:10.17759/chp.2020160204.

Huang Q., Sun J., Tang Y. (2021). Chinese Parents’ Scaffolding and Children’s Initiative in Mother-Child and Father-Child Interactions across Different Types of Problem-solving Activities. Early Education and Development,32, 249-271. doi:10.1080/104092 89.2020.1752591

Hughes C, Lindberg A., Devine R.T. (2018). Autonomy support in toddlerhood: Similarities and contrasts between mothers and fathers. Journal of Family Psychology,32(7), 915. doi:10.1037/fam0000450

Jess M., Hastings R.P., Totsika V. (2017). The construct of maternal positivity in mothers of children with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research,61 (10), 928-938. doi: 10.1111/jir.l2402

Kaale A. et al. (2018). Early interaction in autism spectrum disorder: Mothers’ and children’s behaviours during joint engagement. Child: care, health and development,44 (2), 312-318. doi: 10.1111/cch.l2532

Karabanova O.A., Malofeev N.N. (2019). Education Development Strategy for Children with Disabilities: On the Way to Implementing a Cultural-Historical Approach. Cultural-historical psychology [Kulturno-istoricheskaja psihologija],15 (4), 89-99. doi:10.17759/chp.2019150409. (in Russ.)

Konokotin A.V. (2019). Inclusion of Children with Special Educational Needsand Typically Developing Children in Joint Problem-Solving (With Tasks on Understanding Multiplicative Relationsas an Example). Cultural-historical psychology [Kulturno-istoricheskajapsihologija],15 (4), 79-88. doi:10.17759/chp.2019150408. (in Russ.)

Korepanova I.A., Safronova M.A. (2011). Three Concepts Reflecting the Reality of Child Development: Ability to learn, Zone of Proximal Development and Scaffolding. Cultural-historical psychology [Kulturno-istoricheskaja psihologija],2, 74-83. (in Russ.) 

Meins E., Bureau J.F., Fernyhough C. (2018). Mother-child attachment from infancy to the preschool years: Predicting security and stability. Child Development, 89 (3), 1022-1038. doi: 10.1111/cdev.l2778

Munich Functional Developmental Diagnosis for the First, Second and Third Year of Life(1995). Kerala : Jeevan prakash Child Centre, 332 p.

Nelson J.A. et al. (2019). Relations between characteristics of collaborative and oppositional mother-child conflict. Parenting,19 (3), 203-216. doi:10.1080/15295192.2019.1615794

To cite this article:

Ailamazyan Aida M.. Free dance as a cultural-historical practice of improvisation. // National Psychological Journal 2021. 1. p.175-192. doi: 10.11621/npj.2021.0114

Copied to Clipboard

Copy