ISSN 2079-6617
eISSN 2309-9828
Categorical search for three-dimensional shapes in participants with different level of mathematical expertise

Categorical search for three-dimensional shapes in participants with different level of mathematical expertise

PDF (Rus)

Recieved: 02/03/2020

Accepted: 02/15/2020

Published: 03/31/2020

p.: 57-65

DOI: 10.11621/npj.2020.0106

Keywords: categorical search; extrafoveal analysis; covert attention; pre-attentive processing; three-dimensional shapes; perceptual processes; visual search mechanisms; attention; interindividual differences

Available online: 31.03.2020

To cite this article:

Dreneva Anna A.. Categorical search for three-dimensional shapes in participants with different level of mathematical expertise. // National Psychological Journal 2020. 1. p.57-65. doi: 10.11621/npj.2020.0106

Copied to Clipboard

Copy
Issue 1, 2020

Dreneva Anna A. Lomonosov Moscow State University

Abstract

Background. The paper considers the issue of categorical search for three-dimensional geometric shapes. The results of the previous studies on the real-world objects showed that pre-attentive extrafoveal analysis provides an effective identification of the target, reflecting in the right direction of the first saccade compared with the random search.

Objective. In this study, we investigated the efficacy of extrafoveal analysis during categorical search for pyramids in two groups of participants: mathematicians and psychologists.

Design. 15 mathematicians and 17 psychologists aged 18-25 participated in the study. Each participant performed 144 trialssearching for the target pyramid among the distractor pyramids. In the series 2and 9, any eye movements were prohibited to study covert attention.

Results. The findings of the experiment failed to reveal any differences in the use of extrafoveal analysis and accuracy in both groups. We observed dramatic individual differences, revealed in various degrees of extrafoveal analysis in the search process and in changing the strategy of the task solving. Special series which prohibited eye movements, demonstrated a significant role of covert attention in the categorical search, as well as the possibility and effectiveness of extrafoveal analysis even though while free instruction it could not be used by all participants and all cases.

Conclusion. During categorical search both foveal and extrafoveal processing of stimuli, as well as pre-attentive analysis and covert attention, were simultaneously involved. The dynamics of their interaction was determined by low-level perceptual processes, individual characteristics of the subjects, the task specificity and the instruction.

Tab. 1. Number of correct answers in groups of mathematicians and psychologists

Mathematicians ("experts")

Psychologists ("freshers")

Subject's initials

Number of correct answers (%)

Subject's initials

Number of correct answers (%)

V

58,9

T

50

A1

65,3

A4

50,7

M1

68,5

A5

54,9

A2

68,7

M3

55,2

A3

69,2

I

56,6

Y

69,9

M4

62,5

N

70,6

D2

69,7

E1

72,1

V

74,8

E2

72,9

E3

76,1

D1

74,1

M5

77,8

S

90,2

K2

78

K1

91

A6

81,1

M2

93,1

A7

82,6

K3

82,6

A8

84

D3

85

Average

74,2

Average

70,1

Median

70,6

Median

75,45


Fig. 1. Sample stimulus slide


Fig. 2. Histogram of average FirstT indicators in subjects


Fig. 3. Relationship between the dynamics of the indicator FirstT and the average FirstT in subjects

Acknowledgments

The work was carried out as part of a research project under the RFBR grant “Extrafoveal analysis of visual stimuli given by mathematical concepts: the interaction of descending and ascending processes”. Contract N 18-013-00869

References

Chen X., & Zelinsky G.J. (2006). Real-world visual search is dominated by top-down guidance. Vision research, 46(24), 4118–4133. doi: 10.1016/j. visres.2006.08.008

Chumachenko D.V., & Schwartz A.Yu. (2016). The issue of transforming perceptual processes in training: analysis of studies performed by recording eye movements from the perspective of the activity approach [Psikhologicheskie issledovaniya: elektronnyy nauchnyy zhurnal], 9(49). 12–12. Retrieved from: http://psystudy.ru/num/2016v9n49/1329-chumachenko49

Deubel H. (2008). The time course of presaccadic attention shifts. Psychological research, 72(6), 630. doi: 10.1007/s00426-008-0165-3

Drenyova A.A. et al. (2019). Extrafoveal analysis of categorically defined three-dimensional figures. [Sibirskiy psikhologicheskiy zhurnal], 72, 68–92. 10.17223/17267080/72/4

Eckstein M.P. (2011). Visual search: A retrospective. Journal of vision, 11(5), 14–14. doi: 10.1167/11.5.14

Eckstein M.P., Drescher B.A., & Shimozaki S.S. (2006). Attentional cues in real scenes, saccadic targeting, and Bayesian priors. Psychological science, 17(11), 973–980. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01815.x

Findlay J.M. (1997). Saccade target selection during visual search. Vision research, 37(5), 617–631. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(96)00218-0

Frömer R. et al. (2015). Are individual differences in reading speed related to extrafoveal visual acuity and crowding? PloS one, 10(3), / doi: 10.1371/ journal.pone.0121986

Gandini D., Lemaire P., & Dufau S. (2008). Older and younger adults’ strategies in approximate quantification. Acta psychological, 129(1), 175–189. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.05.009

Gegenfurtner A., Lehtinen E., & Säljö R. (2011). Expertise differences in the comprehension of visualizations: A meta-analysis of eye-tracking research in professional domains. Educational Psychology Review, 23(4), 523–552. doi: 10.1007/s10648-011-9174-7

Goodwin C. (1994). Professional vision. American anthropologist, 96(3), 606–633. doi: 10.1525/aa.1994.96.3.02a00100

Henderson J.M., Weeks Jr P.A., & Hollingworth A. (1999). The effects of semantic consistency on eye movements during complex scene viewing. Journal of experimental psychology: Human perception and performance, 25(1), 210. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.25.1.210

Krichevets A.N. et al. (2017). Opportunities of extrafoveal perception of geometric shapes. [Voprosy psikhologii], 6, 117–128.

Krichevets A.N., Shvarts A.Y., & Chumachenko D.V. (2014). Perceptual action of novices and experts in operating visual representations of a mathematical concept. [Psikhologiya. Zhurnal Vysshey shkoly ekonomiki], 11(3), 55–78.

Posner M.I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 32(1), 3–25. doi: 10.1080/00335558008248231

Radford L. (2010). The eye as a theoretician: Seeing structures in generalizing activities. For the learning of mathematics, 30(2), 2–7. doi: 10.2307/20749442

Rosenholtz R., Huang J., & Ehinger K.A. (2012). Rethinking the role of top-down attention in vision: Effects attributable to a lossy representation in peripheral vision. Frontiers in psychology, 3(13). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00013

Schmidt J., & Zelinsky G.J. (2009). Search guidance is proportional to the categorical specificity of a target cue. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(10), 1904–1914. doi: 10.1080/17470210902853530

Schmidt J., Zelinsky G.J. (2011). Visual search guidance is best after a short delay. Vision Research, 51(6), 535–545. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.01.013

Treisman A. M., & Gelade G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive psychology, 12(1), 97–136. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5

Vickery T.J., King L.W., & Jiang Y. (2005). Setting up the target template in visual search. Journal of Vision, 5(1), 8–8. doi: 10.1167/5.1.8

Williams D.E. et al. (1997). Patterns of eye movements during parallel and serial visual search tasks. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/ Revue canadienne de psychologieexpérimentale, 51(2), 151. doi: 10.1037/1196-1961.51.2.151

Wolfe J.M. et al. (2004). How fast can you change your mind? The speed of top-down guidance in visual search. Vision research, 44(12), 1411–1426. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2003.11.024

Wolfe J.M. (1998). What can 1 million trials tell us about visual search? Psychological Science, 9(1), 33–39. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00006

Yang H., & Zelinsky G.J. (2009). Visual search is guided to categorically-defined targets. Vision research, 49(16), 2095–2103. doi: 10.1016/j. visres.2009.05.017

Zelinsky G.J. et al. (1997). Eye movements reveal the spatiotemporal dynamics of visual search. Psychological science, 8(6), 448–453. doi: 10.1111/j.1467- 9280.1997.tb00459.x

Zelinsky G.J. et al. (2013). Modelling eye movements in a categorical search task. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368(1628). doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0058

Zelinsky G.J., & Schmidt J. (2009). An effect of referential scene constraint on search implies scene segmentation. Visual Cognition, 17(6–7), 1004–1028. doi: 10.1080/13506280902764315

Zelinsky G.J., & Sheinberg D.L. (1997). Eye movements during parallel–serial visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23(1), 244. 10.1037/0096-1523.23.1.244

Zhang L. et al. (2008). SUN: A Bayesian framework for saliency using natural statistics. Journal of vision, 8(7), 32–32. doi: 10.1167/8.7.32

To cite this article:

Dreneva Anna A.. Categorical search for three-dimensional shapes in participants with different level of mathematical expertise. // National Psychological Journal 2020. 1. p.57-65. doi: 10.11621/npj.2020.0106

Copied to Clipboard

Copy